She can’t recall a single famous composer,
but knows the purpose of a viola’s bridge.
She hasn’t flown a plane since 2007, when
viral encephalitis destroyed her hippocampus, the part of the brain used to
form new memories and retrieve old ones. And she couldn’t describe a single
trip she’s ever taken. But in detail, she’ll list the steps needed to keep a
plane from stalling and where to find the rudder controls.
Johns Hopkins University cognitive
scientists say the sharp contrasts in this patient’s memory profile — her
inability to remember facts about pursuits once vital to her life as an artist,
musician and amateur aviator, while clearly remembering facts relevant to
performing in these domains — suggest conventional wisdom about how the brain
stores knowledge is incorrect.
Conventional wisdom about memory firmly
separates declarative knowledge, or memories about facts, from memories for
skills, or “muscle memory.” For instance, a severe amnesiac with muscle memory
might never forget how to ride a bike, but probably couldn’t recall anything
about the Tour de France. But because skilled performance, like playing music
or flying airplanes, requires much more than mere muscle memory, and because
this patient retained it despite losing most other aspects of her declarative
memory, researchers conclude this type of skill-related declarative knowledge
is different.
“There is such a contrast between her not
being able to tell us anything about her former life and not being able to tell
us anything about many aspects of art and music that she once knew well, but
when we ask her to tell us how to do a watercolor, she is articulate and full
of detail,” said Barbara Landau, the Dick and Lydia Todd Professor of
Cognitive Science at Johns Hopkins. “How can you talk about this knowledge of
“how to” as distinct from declarative knowledge? It is declarative knowledge.”
The findings, now online, are due to appear in an upcoming issue of the journal Cognitive Neuropsychology.
The findings, now online, are due to appear in an upcoming issue of the journal Cognitive Neuropsychology.
Before her illness, Lonni Sue Johnson, 64,
was an accomplished artist whose portfolio included six New Yorker magazine
covers. She was also an amateur violist who played in orchestras and chamber
groups and a licensed single-engine airplane pilot who flew more than 400
flights and owned two planes. Her illness left her with severe brain damage and
catastrophic memory impairment, including severe losses of memory about her
previous life and severely restricted ability to learn new facts.
She has very little memory of her past —
not even of her wedding day. She forgets having done something immediately
after doing it. She also has very little memory for general world knowledge,
including facts about the fields in which she once excelled.
To determine whether Johnson’s
“skill-related” memory was preserved despite extensive losses in memory for
general world knowledge, the team tested her on her memory for facts related to
performing four of her former top skills — art, music, flying and driving. They
gave the same tests to people of similar age and experience in those areas, as
well as to people with no experience in them.
The oral tests, of about 80 questions each,
covered information about the techniques, equipment and terminology involved in
performing the various skills. They included queries such as “How might one
remove excess paint when painting with watercolor?” and “How should one touch
the strings of an instrument to produce a harmonic?”
In art and driving, Johnson scored nearly
as high as experts taking the test. In music and aviation, she did not perform
as well, but knew considerably more than the novices.
“Although Johnson had not created
watercolors, had not flown a plane, and had not driven since her illness, she
could still describe how one would go about carrying out these activities,”
said Johns Hopkins cognitive scientist Michael
McCloskey. “These findings suggest that skill-related knowledge can be
spared even with dramatic losses in other kinds of knowledge.”
The team also included first author Emma
Gregory, a former Johns Hopkins post-doctoral fellow, and research assistant
Zoe Ovans, also of Johns Hopkins.
This research was supported by the Brain Science
Institute at Johns Hopkins.
No comments:
Post a Comment